首页 | 官方网站   微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   5493篇
  免费   327篇
  国内免费   8篇
工业技术   5828篇
  2023年   60篇
  2022年   38篇
  2021年   154篇
  2020年   125篇
  2019年   139篇
  2018年   133篇
  2017年   149篇
  2016年   172篇
  2015年   151篇
  2014年   202篇
  2013年   368篇
  2012年   331篇
  2011年   404篇
  2010年   318篇
  2009年   295篇
  2008年   332篇
  2007年   267篇
  2006年   232篇
  2005年   207篇
  2004年   166篇
  2003年   152篇
  2002年   151篇
  2001年   80篇
  2000年   73篇
  1999年   93篇
  1998年   120篇
  1997年   114篇
  1996年   87篇
  1995年   66篇
  1994年   66篇
  1993年   65篇
  1992年   58篇
  1991年   28篇
  1990年   39篇
  1989年   25篇
  1988年   25篇
  1987年   38篇
  1986年   30篇
  1985年   35篇
  1984年   22篇
  1983年   10篇
  1982年   19篇
  1981年   27篇
  1980年   10篇
  1979年   11篇
  1977年   18篇
  1976年   21篇
  1975年   9篇
  1973年   14篇
  1970年   9篇
排序方式: 共有5828条查询结果,搜索用时 203 毫秒
71.
This article explores the practice of "beeping" or "missed calling" between mobile phone users, or calling a number and hanging up before the mobile's owner can pick up the call. Most beeps are requests to call back immediately, but they can also send a pre-negotiated instrumental message such as "pick me up now" or a relational sign, such as "I'm thinking of you." The practice itself is old, with roots in landline behaviors, but it has grown tremendously, particularly in the developing world. Based on interviews with small business owners and university students in Rwanda, the article identifies three kinds of beeps (callback, pre-negotiated instrumental, and relational) and the norms governing their use. It then assesses the significance of the practice using adaptive structuration theory. In concluding, the article contrasts beeping with SMS/text messaging, discusses its implications for increasing access to telecommunications services, and suggests paths for future research.  相似文献   
72.
THE IMPORTANCE OF NEUTRAL EXAMPLES FOR LEARNING SENTIMENT   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
Most research on learning to identify sentiment ignores "neutral" examples, learning only from examples of significant (positive or negative) polarity. We show that it is crucial to use neutral examples in learning polarity for a variety of reasons. Learning from negative and positive examples alone will not permit accurate classification of neutral examples. Moreover, the use of neutral training examples in learning facilitates better distinction between positive and negative examples.  相似文献   
73.
STUDY OBJECTIVE: To validate the utility of a previously reported 3-point limited sampling model (LSM) for determining etoposide area under the curve to infinity (AUC(infinity)). DESIGN: Secondary analysis of data from two clinical trials of etoposide. SETTING: University medical center clinical research center. PATIENTS: Thirty-four patients with different malignancies. INTERVENTIONS: Etoposide was administered as a 2-hour infusion to 34 patients. Serial plasma samples were drawn over 24 hours after the infusion and analyzed for etoposide by high-performance liquid chromatography. MEASUREMENTS AND MAIN RESULTS: The 3-point LSM AUC was compared with a 14-point actual AUC calculated by the linear trapezoidal rule. Actual and predicted AUC(infinity) by the LSM were highly correlated (r=0.97, p<0.0001). The LSM predictions had a mean absolute error of 10.9% (95% CI -14.1, -5.3) and a mean error of -9.7% (95% CI 6.9, 14.9). Nine patients with poor AUC(infinity) estimations by the LSM (error > 12%) tended to have abnormally low or high peak concentrations. CONCLUSION: Our findings suggest the development of more robust LSM using other techniques, such as pharmacostatistical models, that can accommodate a greater degree of pharmacokinetic variability.  相似文献   
74.
Immunocytochemistry and retrograde labeling were used to define the thalamic projections of calbindin- and parvalbumin-containing cells in superficial layers of the rat's superior colliculus (SC). Quantitative analysis revealed that 90.8 +/- 2.2% (mean +/- standard deviation) of the calbindin-immunoreactive neurons in the stratum griseum superficiale (SGS) projected to the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus (LGNd) and that 91.3 +/- 4.3% of calbindin-immunoreactive neurons in the stratum opticum (SO) projected to the lateral posterior nucleus (LP). In contrast, only 17.3 +/- 2.5% of parvalbumin-immunoreactive neurons in the SGS were found to project to the LGNd and 16.5 +/- 3.1% of the parvalbumin-immunoreactive SO cells were retrogradely labeled after LP injections. Few of the parvalbumin-immunoreactive neurons in either the SGS (7.2 +/- 2.5%) or the SO (9.2 +/- 2.5%) were GABA positive. The retrograde-labeling results suggest that parvalbumin-immunoreactive neurons in the rat's SO and SGS may either be primarily interneurons or have descending projections, while calbindin-containing cells are primarily thalamic projection neurons. These results are consistent with data from other rodents, but almost exactly the opposite of data that have been reported for the cat for these same populations of SC projection neurons. Such interspecies differences raise questions regarding the functional importance of expressing one calcium-binding protein versus another in a specific neuronal population.  相似文献   
75.
Semantic priming in word pronunciation was examined at 5 stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs) in 75 medicated and 25 unmedicated people with schizophrenia (SCZ) and in 10 depressed and 28 normal controls. At SOAs  相似文献   
76.
AIMS: Raised lipoprotein(a) concentrations are considered to be a risk factor for atherothrombotic diseases. We examined whether baseline concentrations were a risk factor for an adverse outcome in patients admitted with acute coronary syndromes. METHODS AND RESULTS: Five hundred and nineteen patients admitted with suspected acute coronary syndromes were studied and followed prospectively for a median of 3 years. The prognostic significance of a baseline lipoprotein(a) concentration of > or = 30 mg x dl(-1) or lower for subsequent cardiac death was assessed in patients with myocardial infarction (266) and unstable angina (197) and compared with other variables in regression models. In patients with myocardial infarction, a baseline lipoprotein(a) concentration of > or =30 mg x dl(-1) was associated with a 62% increase in subsequent cardiac death compared to the lower concentration group (29.8% vs 18.6%, Log rank P=0.04). In a multivariate regression model a baseline lipoprotein(a) concentration of > or = 30 mg x dl(-1) retained its significance as an independent predictor of cardiac death (P=0.037). In patients with unstable angina, baseline concentrations of > or = 7.9 mg x dl(-1) were found to be significant predictors of cardiac death in univariate (P=0.021) and multivariate (P=0.035) regression models. CONCLUSION: Baseline lipoprotein(a) concentrations in patients admitted with acute coronary syndromes are associated with an increased risk of cardiac death. For patients with myocardial infarction a concentration of > or = 30 mg x dl(-1) appears appropriate as a risk discriminator; for patients admitted with unstable angina, however, much lower concentrations of lipoprotein(a) appear to be prognostically important.  相似文献   
77.
Methanolic extracts prepared from the leaves of Lantana camara have been found to inhibit human thrombin. An assay, in which thrombin activity is measured as a function of clot formation from fibrinogen, was used to guide the fractionation and purification of five principal active constituents (1-5), which were all characterized as 5,5-trans-fused cyclic lactone-containing euphane triterpenes.  相似文献   
78.
Flow experience, the degree to which a person feels involved in a particular activity, is an important influence on human–computer interaction. Building on Guo and Poole’s (2009) model of flow experience in Web navigation, and van Schaik and Ling's (in press) cognitive-experiential approach to modelling interaction experience, this research demonstrates the crucial role of the preconditions of flow experience in human–computer interaction. In an experiment, the preconditions of flow experience – but not flow experience proper – mediated the effects of artefact complexity, task complexity and intrinsic motivation (as a situation-specific trait) on both flow and task outcome. However, preconditions did not predict overall artefact evaluation. Within a staged model of flow experience, the broader implications of this work for human–computer interaction are explored.  相似文献   
79.
Stereoscopic depth cues improve depth perception and increase immersion within virtual environments (VEs). However, improper display of these cues can distort perceived distances and directions. Consider a multi-user VE, where all users view identical stereoscopic images regardless of physical location. In this scenario, cues are typically customized for one "leader" equipped with a head-tracking device. This user stands at the center of projection (CoP) and all other users ("followers") view the scene from other locations and receive improper depth cues. This paper examines perceived depth distortion when viewing stereoscopic VEs from follower perspectives and the impact of these distortions on collaborative spatial judgments. Pairs of participants made collaborative depth judgments of virtual shapes viewed from the CoP or after displacement forward or backward. Forward and backward displacement caused perceived depth compression and expansion, respectively, with greater compression than expansion. Furthermore, distortion was less than predicted by a ray-intersection model of stereo geometry. Collaboration times were significantly longer when participants stood at different locations compared to the same location, and increased with greater perceived depth discrepancy between the two viewing locations. These findings advance our understanding of spatial distortions in multi-user VEs, and suggest a strategy for reducing distortion.  相似文献   
80.
A Gaussian sum filter (GSF) with component extended Kalman filters (EKF) is proposed as an approach to localizing an autonomous vehicle in an urban environment with limited GPS availability. The GSF uses vehicle‐relative vision‐based measurements of known map features coupled with inertial navigation solutions to accomplish localization in the absence of GPS. The vision‐based measurements have multimodal measurement likelihood functions that are well represented as weighted sums of Gaussian densities. The GSF is used because of its ability to represent the posterior distribution of the vehicle pose with better efficiency (fewer terms, less computational complexity) than a corresponding bootstrap particle filter with various numbers of particles because of the interaction with measurement hypothesis tests. The expectation‐maximization algorithm is used off line to determine the representational efficiency of the particle filter in terms of an effective number of Gaussian densities. In comparison, the GSF, which uses an iterative condensation procedure after each iteration of the filter to maintain real‐time capabilities, is shown through a series of in‐depth empirical studies to more accurately maintain a representation of the posterior distribution than the particle filter using 37 min of recorded data from Cornell University's autonomous vehicle driven in an urban environment, including a 32 min GPS blackout. © 2012 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司    京ICP备09084417号-23

京公网安备 11010802026262号